Jacobsetal.article.pdf

    What individual and contextual factorsexplain what happens to offenders whohave been sentenced to death? An execution

    clearly is the most severe legal punishment,

    but no systematic research on the combined

    political and individual factors that determine

    which death row inmates will be executed

    apparently exists. A few studies describe what

    happens to these offenders after sentencing

    (Aarons 1998; Liebman, Fagan, and West

    2000), yet there are almost no systematic

    investigations on the most important deter-

    minants of executions. Studies by Spurr (2002)

    and Blume and Eisenberg (1999) are partial

    exceptions, but these studies focus only on

    offender characteristics and ignore environ-

    mental conditions. Although studies repeatedly

    show that victim race is the most important

    determinant of death sentences, we do not

    know if this factor influences the fate of

    offenders on death row because no investiga-

    tions have determined whether this account

    explains executions.

    The factors that influence execution prob-

    abilities are of interest partly because there are

    such substantial disparities in this outcome.

    Largely as a result of the appeals process that

    occurs after a death sentence, less than 10

    percent of all offenders on death row ulti-

    Who Survives on Death Row? An Individual and Contextual Analysis

    David Jacobs Zhenchao Qian

    The Ohio State University The Ohio State University

    Jason T. Carmichael Stephanie L. Kent

    McGill University Cleveland State University

    What are the relationships between death row offender attributes, social arrangements,

    and executions? Partly because public officials control executions, theorists view this

    sanction as intrinsically political. Although the literature has focused on offender

    attributes that lead to death sentences, the post-sentencing stage is at least as important.

    States differ sharply in their willingness to execute and less than 10 percent of those

    given a death sentence are executed. To correct the resulting problems with censored

    data, this study uses a discrete-time event history analysis to detect the individual and

    state-level contextual factors that shape execution probabilities. The findings show that

    minority death row inmates convicted of killing whites face higher execution

    probabilities than other capital offenders. Theoretically relevant contextual factors with

    explanatory power include minority presence in nonlinear form, political ideology, and

    votes for Republican presidential candidates. Inasmuch as there is little or no systematic

    research on the individual and contextual factors that influence execution probabilities,

    these findings fill important gaps in the literature.

    AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW, 2007, VOL. 72 (August:610–632)

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 610

    Direct cor respondence to David Jacobs,

    Department of Sociology, 300 Bricker Hall, 190

    Nor th Oval Mall, The Ohio State University,

    Columbus, OH 43210 ([email protected]). We

    thank Douglas Berman for his valuable advice on

    criminal procedure law applied to the death penalty

    and Ruth Peterson for her comments. We are indebt-

    ed to Dan Tope for his research assistance. We also

    thank Ohio State colleagues for their comments in

    presentations at the law school, the political science

    department, the Criminal Justice Research Center,

    colleagues at the University of Cincinnati School of

    Criminal Justice, the editors, and the referees. All data

    used in this study and in the analyses discussed in the

    text but not shown are available on request. This

    research was supported by NSF grant #0417736.

    mately are executed (Liebman et al. 2000).1

    There is great variation in the duration of this

    process as well, apparently because death penal-

    ty states differ sharply in their willingness to

    execute. In many reluctant jurisdictions capital

    offenders can spend well over two decades on

    death row, but other states execute in far less

    time. To assess the determinants of these legal

    decisions about who will live and who will die,

    we test theoretically based explanations using

    an event history approach to discover the fac-

    tors that influence post-death sentence execution

    likelihoods.

    Trial court studies on the offender attributes

    that lead to death sentences show that offend-

    ers who kill whites are far more likely to receive

    this sentence (Baldus and Woodworth 2003;

    Dodge et al. 1990; Paternoster 1991). Yet

    whether victim race continues to explain the

    fate of condemned prisoners after they have

    been sentenced remains a complete mystery.

    There are good reasons to think that this factor

    will continue to matter. Yet it is equally plausi-

    ble that the appellate court decisions that large-

    ly determine death row outcomes are unaffected

    by this consideration that, of course, should not

    be relevant. In any event, both theoretical con-

    siderations and concerns about equity make

    this relationship between victim race and exe-

    cution probabilities a critical issue.

    This article will provide important evidence

    about whether the death penalty is adminis-

    tered impartially. But our primary goal is to

    refine theories of punishment by using a com-

    prehensive approach to gauge the explanatory

    power of both individual and contextual effects.

    It is unlikely that judges and the political offi-

    cials who decide which death row inmates will

    be executed are unaffected by their political

    environment, especially because this punish-

    ment is such an intensely moral issue. In part

    because some death row offenders face far lower

    execution probabilities than those in less lenient

    jurisdictions (Liebman et al. 2000), we gauge the

    effects of the sociopolitical environment as well

    as offender and victim attributes.

    There are strong reasons for such a com-

    bined approach. Two isolated traditions have

    coexisted in the literature. Many studies use

    individual data to explain trial court sentencing,

    but others rely on aggregate data to study addi-

    tional criminal justice outcomes. State or nation-

    al attributes have been used to explain shifts in

    incarceration rates (Jacobs and Carmichael

    2001; Jacobs and Helms 1996; Stucky, Heimer,

    and Lang 2005; Sutton 2000; Western 2006).

    The urban conditions that explain police depart-

    ment size (Jacobs 1979; Kent and Jacobs 2005),

    arrest rates (Brown and Warner 1992), or the use

    of deadly force by the police (Jacobs and

    O’Brien 1998) have been researched as well. Yet

    except for a few studies that assess how com-

    munity and individual determinants combine

    to affect the sentencing of non-capital offend-

    ers (Helms and Jacobs 2002; Myers and Talarico

    1987), there is little research on the combined

    effects of individual and contextual determi-

    nants. But the many results based on aggregate

    data showing that context is a strong determi-

    nant of multiple criminal justice outcomes make

    it difficult to believe that such environmental

    factors do not influence post-sentencing deci-

    sions about executions.

    We therefore use an integrated theoretical

    approach that emphasizes political explanations

    and the racial accounts in earlier conflict stud-

    ies (Turk 1969). An execution is an intrinsical-

    ly political act. Foucault (1977) views executions

    as rituals designed to enhance political power by

    reminding potential miscreants of the state’s

    vast coercive resources. But there are more con-

    crete reasons for studying political effects. Most

    researchers who first tested conflict explanations

    hypothesized that larger and therefore more

    threatening minority populations would increase

    support for repressive law and order measures.

    Citizens threatened by expansions in a minori-

    ty presence often react by demanding harsh

    criminal justice policies. Because criminal jus-

    tice agencies are operated by the state, this pres-

    sure has to be directed at political officials. By

    WHO SURVIVES ON DEATH ROW?—–611

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 611

    1 Local trial courts sentence, but appeals are han-

    dled by higher state and federal courts. The first two

    capital appeals typically are decided by state appel-

    late courts. Condemned offenders then can petition

    the federal courts. About 41 percent of all death sen-

    tences are reversed on first state appeal and about 9.5

    percent are reversed in the second. About 40 percent

    of those who then seek federal relief are successful

    (Liebman et al. 2000). Most of the remainder are exe-

    cuted. But a few receive executive clemency, some

    die before execution, and a few are removed from

    death row for miscellaneous reasons. Almost all of

    the condemned who obtain appellate relief are resen-

    tenced to long prison terms.

    assessing the political factors that result from

    added demands for this severe punishment, we

    seek to broaden the conflict approach to pun-

    ishment.

    This article therefore offers the promise of

    filling many important gaps in the sparse liter-

    ature on post-death sentence execution proba-

    bilities by using a survival analysis that adjusts

    for censoring and assesses both offender and

    political characteristics. The multiple advan-

    tages that result from the inclusion of both indi-

    vidual and contextual factors suggest that this

    analysis will provide an accurate picture of the

    post-sentencing death penalty process. Results

    based on models that assess many explanations

    are most accurate (Johnston 1984, see note 11),

    but such an inclusive approach means that the

    theoretical section cannot focus on only a few

    explanations.

    THEORY

    Inasmuch as scholars claim that race continues

    to have powerful effects on U.S. politics

    (Goldfield 1997; Jacobs and Tope 2007; Key

    1949), and because the administration of the

    death penalty is such an intense political issue,

    racial politics provide the primary conceptual

    basis for this analysis. Most research on the

    death sentence focuses on the race of individ-

    ual offenders and their victims. We assess such

    micro minority accounts by gauging the

    explanatory power of various offender-victim

    minority-majority combinations, but we fill

    contextual gaps in the literature by analyzing the

    influence of minority threat in the political envi-

    ronments in which these decisions are made.

    Ideology also should matter as this penalty is

    such an intensely moral issue and since we

    study it in the most direct of all large democra-

    cies. In contrast to other nations, U.S. politicians

    routinely encourage citizens to vote on the basis

    of their views about capital punishment. Another

    closely related account suggests that the

    parochial interests of politicians influence exe-

    cutions. Tactical rhetoric that stresses the deprav-

    ity of a minority underclass and the need for

    harsh measures should affect support for this

    penalty (Beckett 1997) and its probability.

    We present the theoretical foundation for

    four interrelated proposition sets. First, we dis-

    cuss the conceptual basis for micro-level racial

    hypotheses derived from the literature on sen-

    tencing. Second, we present theoretically based

    hypotheses about the contextual effects of

    minority threat, political ideology, and parti-

    san politics. At the end of this section, we pre-

    sent justifications for additional explanations for

    execution probabilities.

    INDIVIDUAL EXPLANATIONS: EXTRA-LEGAL

    MICRO RACIAL ACCOUNTS AND LEGAL

    FACTORS

    If penal measures are best explained by their

    interrelationships with other social arrange-

    ments rather than by their alleged legal pur-

    poses (Garland 1990:91), it would be surprising

    if the most important U.S. social division did not

    influence the administration of the most severe

    legal punishment. A conceptual grounding in

    racial politics therefore should provide the best

    theoretical foundation for a study that analyzes

    the administration of the death penalty. In the-

    oretical essays Wacquant (2000, 2001) uses

    arguments about social impurity and taboo to

    show how the Jim Crow racial caste system

    persists in socially altered but less conspicuous

    forms in the contemporary U.S. criminal justice

    system. To discover if such racial considera-

    tions still affect decisions about legal punish-

    ments, the question that motivates almost all of

    the many sentencing studies is whether the trial

    courts treat minorities the same as nonminori-

    ties (Chiricos and Crawford 1995; Walker,

    Spohn, and DeLone 1996; Zatz 1987).

    Findings about non-capital sentences, and

    those from the less ample literature on the fac-

    tors that account for death sentences, should

    provide the best available empirical basis for

    selecting the individual factors that explain

    which condemned prisoners will be executed.

    Despite their number, only a bare majority of the

    non-capital sentencing studies show that the

    trial courts give harsher sentences to minorities,

    but almost as many careful investigations do not

    find such biases (Chiricos and Crawford 1995;

    Walker et al. 1996). Studies on the links between

    offender race and death sentences are even less

    likely to suggest that white offenders are treat-

    ed with greater leniency than minorities (Baldus

    and Woodworth 2003; Dodge et al. 1990;

    Paternoster 1991). In light of the systematic

    discrimination faced by African Americans,

    however, we follow precedent and hypothesize

    that: The likelihood of executions should be

    612—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 612

    greater for African Americans on death row. In

    some jurisdictions Hispanics are the most threat-

    ening minority that faces severe discrimina-

    tion. It is equally plausible that: Hispanics on

    death row should face higher execution proba-

    bilities than whites.

    Offenders convicted of killing whites are par-

    ticularly likely to be sentenced to death (Baldus

    and Woodworth 2003; Dodge et al. 1990;

    Paternoster 1991), especially if the offender is

    black (Baldus and Woodworth 2003). Wacquant

    (2000) provides one theoretical foundation for

    this persistent association when he claims that

    harsh legal punishments continue to be used to

    maintain the “symbolic distance needed to pre-

    vent the odium of ‘amalgamation’ with [minori-

    ties] considered inferior, rootless, and vile” (p.

    380). The ultimate symbolic assault on such a

    caste system occurs when an underclass minor-

    ity kills a white. In this research we discover if

    this account that explains death sentences so

    well also explains execution probabilities.

    Two causal paths seem most plausible. In

    contrast to the great majority of homicides,

    media outlets focus on interracial murders, par-

    ticularly if the victim is white (Bandes 2004,

    Lipschultz and Hilt 2002). This increased cov-

    erage is critical as victories in well-publicized

    capital trials often help politically ambitious

    prosecutors reach higher office. But such tri-

    umphs must be protected. Prosecutors there-

    fore have strong reasons to vigorously oppose

    capital appeals that may jeopardize legal victo-

    ries that are likely to further their political

    careers.2 Even if other facilitative conditions

    are absent, the media’s focus on those murders

    in which a white was killed by a minority puts

    added pressure on state appellate court justices

    to rule against these death row offenders when

    they appeal (Liebman, Fagan, and West 2002).

    And disregarding these pressures can be cost-

    ly. State judges who ignored intense public sup-

    port for particular executions and granted

    appellate relief to such petitioners have lost

    their seats in retention elections, even though

    only the incumbent appears on retention elec-

    tion ballots (Brace and Hall 1997; Bright and

    Keenan 1995). Hence: African American or

    Hispanic offenders convicted of murdering a

    white should be less likely than other offenders

    to avoid the death chamber.

    Most studies of the determinants of non-cap-

    ital sentencing that assess the effects of gender

    find that in comparison to females, trial courts

    give less lenient sentences to males (Bickle and

    Peterson 1991). This finding may be partially

    based on a failure to control for the ways offend-

    ers participated in their crimes. Women con-

    victed of robbery, for instance, often do not

    engage in the violence associated with this

    felony. Inasmuch as their involvement is less

    pernicious, such offenders receive lighter sen-

    tences than their male associates. It nevertheless

    is reasonable to expect that chivalrous inclina-

    tions should reduce female execution probabil-

    ities, so: Women on death row should be less

    likely to be executed than males. Finally, find-

    ings show that offenders with prior convictions

    are sentenced more severely by trial courts. This

    practice is reasonable as repeat offenders are

    likely to pose a greater threat to the communi-

    ty after their release or to guards and other

    inmates while they are imprisoned. Hence:

    Execution probabilities will be greater for those

    condemned prisoners with previous convictions.

    CONTEXTUAL EXPLANATIONS: RACIAL

    THREAT, POLITICAL IDEOLOGY, AND

    PARTISANSHIP

    The multiple decisions that ultimately produce

    an execution do not occur in a social vacuum.

    A few studies of sentencing decisions for non-

    capital crimes productively treat the trial courts

    as complex organizations. Yet analyses of con-

    textual forces external to organizations have

    sharply increased our understanding of organi-

    zational behavior (Perrow 1986; Scott 1987).

    The multiple findings based on aggregate data

    that provide such robust explanations for crim-

    inal justice outcomes and the strong organiza-

    tion-environment relationships so often

    uncovered in the organizational literature point

    WHO SURVIVES ON DEATH ROW?—–613

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 613

    2 After a death sentence, local prosecutors can

    have important effects on the subsequent appeals. For

    example, their delays in filing for an execution date

    slow appeals. In fact, any failure to act promptly

    favors condemned offenders as delayed appeals are

    not as likely to be vigorously contested. For this and

    other reasons, the local prosecutor who won an ini-

    tial death sentence verdict often plays an important

    role in resisting subsequent appeals, although this role

    may be informal. Hence, prosecutor commitment to

    efforts to resist death row appeals should influence

    execution probabilities.

    in the same direction. Both research streams

    suggest that legal decision makers who are

    embedded in political environments do not

    ignore such conditions when they decide if an

    execution will occur.

    RACIAL MIX. The fierce U.S. disputes about

    race in the past probably make this fissure the

    most resilient and influential division in con-

    temporary U.S. politics (Goldfield 1997; Jacobs

    and Tope 2007; Key 1949). A majority’s eth-

    nocentric views and that group’s inclination to

    view minorities as trespassers enhance such a

    group’s presumption that they should retain

    exclusive claims over important rights and priv-

    ileges (Blalock 1967; Blumer 1958; Bobo and

    Hutchings 1996). Hostility and entrenched

    beliefs about a majority’s “rightful” position

    are solidified by the political struggles that

    occur when minority groups seek to alter these

    arrangements (Blumer 1958). According to

    threat theorists, when large minority popula-

    tions endanger their dominance, whites often

    react by supporting law and order measures that

    at least indirectly target these minorities.

    Findings are supportive. Racist views are

    more widespread in cities with more black res-

    idents (Fosset and Kiecolt 1989; Quillian 1996;

    Taylor 1998). An enhanced minority presence

    produces added votes for anti-minority candi-

    dates (Giles and Buckner 1993; Giles and Hertz

    1994; Heer 1959) who are likely to endorse

    harsh criminal punishments. With crime rates

    held constant, Liska, Lawrence, and Sanchirico

    (1982) and Quillian and Pager (2001) find that

    fear of crime is greater in cities or neighbor-

    hoods with more black residents. Larger minor-

    ity populations lead to additional police officers

    (Jacobs 1979; Kent and Jacobs 2005). Other

    findings show that the death penalty is likely to

    be legal in states with the highest percentages

    of African American residents (Jacobs and

    Carmichael 2002), while the number of death

    sentences is greater in states with the largest

    African American populations (Jacobs,

    Carmichael, and Kent 2005).

    These results suggest that severe punishments

    will increase in areas after a growth in minori-

    ty presence. Yet if minority proportions expand

    and their political influence becomes sufficient,

    the positive relationship between this threat and

    punitiveness may reverse. All governors, almost

    all local prosecutors, and most state appellate

    justices are elected. Expansions in African

    American or Hispanic proportions past a thresh-

    old should give these minorities enough votes

    to influence decisions about executions. Hence:

    The relationship between the percentage of

    blacks and execution probabilities should be

    positive if minority presence is modest, but after

    this percentage reaches a threshold, this asso-

    ciation should become negative and executions

    should diminish. For this sign reversal to occur,

    a minority need not outnumber other groups.

    Minority size need only reach the point where

    their votes may help decide elections, and this

    proportion can be modest if other voting blocs

    are evenly matched. This logic and nonlinear

    findings about death sentence frequency (Jacobs

    et al. 2005) suggest that an inverted U-shaped

    relationship between African American pres-

    ence and executions will be present.

    The relationship between Hispanic presence

    and executions may take a different nonlinear

    form. In many non-southwestern states, the pro-

    portion of Hispanic residents is minute. The

    median percentage of Hispanics was 2.4 percent

    in the sampled states, yet the same statistic for

    blacks was 6.8 percent or 2.9 times greater. As

    the Hispanic population was so modest in so

    many states, it is plausible that this population

    must reach a threshold size before whites see

    Hispanics as sufficiently threatening. Hence:

    The nonlinear relationship between the per-

    centage of Hispanic residents and execution

    probabilities should become increasingly pos-

    itive only after the comparative size of this eth-

    nic minority reaches a level sufficient to threaten

    majority Anglos.

    POLITICAL IDEOLOGY. Claims that ideology

    helps shape legal penalties are especially com-

    pelling when U.S. sanctions are at issue, as this

    nation is such an exceptionally direct democracy

    (Savelsberg 1994; Whitman 2003). The result-

    ing close voter control over criminal punish-

    ments, which are decided by bureaucratic

    experts in the less direct European democracies,

    ought to make mass ideologies crucial when

    such intensely moral decisions about who will

    die must be made. Images of evil and the result-

    ing beliefs about the most appropriate punish-

    ments that stem from these assumptions about

    human nature are a foundational component of

    political ideologies. Conservatives often see

    crime as resulting from freely made but amoral

    614—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 614

    choices (Lacey 1988). If such presumptions are

    correct, increases in expected costs should be

    effective. Many conservatives therefore stress

    the irreversibility and the deterrent effects of

    executions. Such views about the efficacy of

    incapacitation and deterrence provide the basis

    for empirically dubious conservative claims

    that a few executions will protect many innocent

    victims from criminal brutality.3

    Liberals instead see criminal acts as imposed

    by circumstances (Garland 2001; Thorne 1990).

    These acts result from noxious environmental

    conditions such as poverty or discrimination.

    Liberals view policies that alleviate these inju-

    rious conditions (Taylor, Walton, and Young

    1973) and therapeutic efforts to resocialize

    offenders (Garland 2001) as the best remedies.

    In contrast to conservatives, liberal survey

    respondents are far less likely to support the

    death penalty (Lakoff 1996; Langworthy and

    Whitehead 1986). Findings corroborate these

    claims as they show that the most liberal states

    are unlikely to legalize this punishment (Jacobs

    and Car michael 2002). It follows that:

    Executions should not be as probable where

    liberal views predominate, as prosecutors and

    appellate justices should be less likely to

    endorse this penalty in these jurisdictions.

    POLITICAL PARTISANSHIP. Because they seek

    outcomes that help the prosperous, political

    parties closer to the right face election obstacles.

    These parties, for example, usually choose tax

    policies that benefit their affluent core sup-

    porters at the expense of the less affluent (Allen

    and Campbell 1994). Yet prosperous voters are

    in the minority. Because such parties have a

    smaller voter base than their rivals, conservatives

    often use law and order claims to appeal to less

    affluent voters who are more likely to be crime

    victims and who often live where such risks

    are greater. Officials in the Nixon and first

    Bush campaigns for the presidency admit that

    they emphasized this issue to attract anti-minor-

    ity voters.4 By focusing on street crime and

    other social problems readily blamed on under-

    class minorities, Republicans won elections by

    using this “wedge” issue to gain sufficient votes

    from less prosperous citizens. Multiple findings

    show that Republican (Jacobs and Carmichael

    2001; Stucky et al. 2005; Western 2006) or con-

    servative political strength (Sutton 2000) led

    to severe criminal justice outcomes. Because

    capital punishment has been an important issue

    in many state political campaigns (Constanzo

    1997) and because Jacobs and Carmichael

    (2002) find that this punishment is likely to be

    legal in states with the strongest Republican

    parties, we expect that greater Republican polit-

    ical strength in a state should increase execution

    probabilities.

    Republican campaigns for the presidency

    have relied on and probably accentuated

    (Beckett 1997) mass perceptions about the links

    between purportedly venal underclass life styles

    and lawlessness. Findings show that votes for the

    f irst of these presidents who successfully

    exploited public views about the linkages

    between race and crime (Nixon in 1968) help

    explain how quickly states relegalized capital

    punishment after the 1976 court decisions that

    WHO SURVIVES ON DEATH ROW?—–615

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 615

    3 Careful reviews of the multiple empirical stud-

    ies on this issue conducted by legal scholars (Zimring

    and Hawkins 1986), criminologists (Hood 1998;

    Paternoster 1991), sociologists (Bailey and Peterson

    1999), and economists (Donohue and Wolfers 2005;

    Levitt 2002) conclude that the death penalty has no

    discernable general deterrent effects beyond those

    imposed by long prison terms. This list of skeptics

    about the deterrent effects of executions includes a

    scholar (Levitt 2002) who has published multiple

    findings showing that imprisonment and other poli-

    cies designed to control crime are effective deterrents.

    4 A participant described Nixon’s 1968 campaign:

    “We’ll go after the racists. That subliminal appeal to

    the anti-black voter was always present in Nixon’s

    statements and speeches” (Ehrlichman 1982:233).

    Other vivid examples occurred in the 1988 Bush

    campaign against Dukakis. Republicans ran an adver-

    tisement declaring, “‘Dukakis not only opposed the

    death penalty, he allowed first-degree murderers to

    have weekend passes from prison.’.|.|. [as the] clear-

    ly black [offender]—Willie Horton stared dully into

    the camera.” They next released an advertisement fea-

    turing a victim. “‘Mike Dukakis and Willie Horton

    changed our lives forever .|.|. Horton broke into our

    home. For twelve hours, I was beaten, slashed, and

    terrorized. My wife Angie was brutally raped’”

    (Carter 1996:76–77). This emphasis did not abate. In

    a House debate in 1994 “29 Republican[s] .|.|. spoke

    derisively about midnight basketball .|.|. character-

    izing the program as ‘hugs for thugs’” (Hurwitz and

    Peffley 2005:99–100).

    forced changes in these statutes (Jacobs and

    Carmichael 2002). Incarceration rates also were

    higher after more voters supported a Republican

    law and order presidential candidate (Weidner

    and Frase 2003). Hence: Death row inmates

    should be less likely to avoid the death cham-

    ber in states in which more voters support

    Republican presidential candidates because

    prosecutors, appellate justices, and governors

    will face stronger pressures to allow executions

    in these jurisdictions.

    ADDITIONAL CONTROLS

    Murder is the most threatening crime. Execution

    probabilities therefore should be greater in states

    with higher murder rates. Following Durkheim’s

    emphasis on the determinants of restitutive law,

    states with a larger and more diverse population

    and an enhanced division of labor should not be

    as likely to use this most severe punishment.

    This perspective also suggests that solidarity

    should matter. Migration interferes with social

    cohesion. Outsiders inspire hostility and fear, but

    their absence strengthens bonds and intragroup

    empathetic feelings (Hale 1996). Citizens in

    states with few outsiders therefore should not be

    as willing to support executions. This factor

    accounts for the legality of the death penalty

    (Jacobs and Carmichael 2002), so executions

    should be less likely when most residents were

    born in the states where they now live.

    Research on imprisonment has focused on the

    Marxist view that punishment is used to control

    the supply of labor (Rusche and Kirchheimer

    1939). Many researchers have assessed the link

    between unemployment and incarceration, but the

    results are mixed. A review (Chiricos and Delone

    1992) shows that about 60 percent of the 147

    associations between unemployment and impris-

    onments are significant. Despite such mixed

    results, we expect that high joblessness will

    enhance execution probabilities because the pros-

    perous may view the unemployed as a threat or

    because high unemployment enhances resent-

    ments against criminals and accentuates demands

    for harsh sanctions. Yet the unemployment rate

    may have to reach a threshold before it matters,

    so we test a nonlinear relationship between this

    variable and execution probabilities. Primarily as

    a result of the multiple state and federal appeals,

    expensive expert testimony, and the other costs

    required for due process in decisions that may end

    a life, executions are far more expensive than

    alternatives such as life imprisonment. States

    with a superior tax base should be more likely to

    use this expensive punishment. Finally, to see if

    the unique arrangements in the South influence

    death row outcomes, we also control for this

    region.

    METHODS

    ESTIMATION

    Our aim is to discover how offender attributes

    and the political and social characteristics of the

    states affect post-sentencing execution likeli-

    hoods. To test these hypotheses, we use an event

    history approach. This procedure provides a

    remedy for the censoring that occurs because

    offenders face different execution risks. Some

    offenders remained on death row after 2001, so

    this right-censored group was in danger of being

    executed after the end of the observation peri-

    od. The other right-censoring occurs when

    offenders were removed from death row large-

    ly as a result of successful appeals. Event his-

    tory analysis uses the information from cases

    with incomplete duration who were not exe-

    cuted to avoid the bias that would occur if these

    censored cases were not included.5 We employ

    discrete-time logit models to predict execution

    probabilities. Our first model will assess the

    influence of individual factors. This model takes

    the form:

    Pijtlog ( 1 – Pijt) = atd +

    M

    Sm=1

    bmXmi (1)

    where Pijt is the conditional probability of exe-

    cution for death row offender i in state j at time

    616—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 616

    5 Because event history analysis takes advantage

    of all available information, we include death row

    offenders near the end of the analysis period although

    such offenders are not as likely to be executed.

    Offenders still on death row are right-censored regard-

    less of how long they have been on death row, but our

    estimation approach will not be biased by this or

    other forms of censoring. Note that selection biases

    should not be problematic because we only explain

    what happens to offenders sentenced to death. We

    make no claims about whether those who received a

    less severe punishment than death would have dif-

    ferent likelihoods of being executed if they had been

    sentenced to death.

    t given that the execution has not already

    occurred to that individual prior to time t. t is

    years from death sentence. The log odds of exe-

    cution at time t is a function of a set of time-con-

    stant covariates as well as a set of temporal

    dummies (td) to account for time dependence

    (atd).6 The individual level covariates entered in

    this model include offender race, ethnicity, gen-

    der, prior convictions, and victim race. The

    models also include interactions between

    offender and victim race. We assess the explana-

    tory power of state-level contextual effects by

    using variables such as the percentage of a

    state’s vote for Republican candidates in time-

    varying form. The subsequent and more exhaus-

    tive models therefore take the following form:

    Pijtlog ( 1 – Pijt) =

    atd +

    M

    Sm=1

    bmXmi +

    N

    Sn=1

    bnXnj(t–1) (2)

    where the conditional probability of execution

    at time t is a function of a set of individual con-

    demned prisoner factors as well as state char-

    acteristics at time t-1. Death row offenders may

    be treated similarly within a state with the same

    death penalty provisions and the same officials

    deciding appeals. To adjust for this possible

    departure from statistical independence, we

    report z-values corrected for within-state cor-

    related errors with a cluster procedure (Rogers

    1993).

    SAMPLE

    Most offender information was taken from

    (ICPSR study 3958) “Capital Punishment in

    the United States” (CPUS) from 1973 to 2002

    (this source is limited to those years). This

    source gives state, date of removal from death

    row, and reasons for removal, but it does not

    contain data on victim race. For all offenders

    who were executed, race of the victim, offend-

    er race, state, and execution date are available

    from the Death Penalty Information Center.7

    To obtain victim race we merged this data into

    the CPUS file by matching execution date and

    race of executed offenders.8

    To obtain race of victim for former death

    row offenders removed from death row before

    execution or for offenders still on death row, we

    had to use another source. The Supplemental

    Homicide Reports (SHR) contain information

    on offense date, victim race, offender race, and

    age. We obtain victim race by matching SHR

    data with offender data from state correction

    department records by f irst using date of

    offense, and then offender race and age, but

    only 16 states could or would provide offense

    date. We merge the offender data with the CPUS

    data using date of sentence, offender race, and

    age. This matching process yields victim race

    for 1,012 current and former death row offend-

    ers who were not executed in 16 states. To cap-

    ture the effects of this explanatory variable, the

    analysis must be restricted to 16 states, but these

    states are selected by a presumably random

    process based on whether a state provides date

    of offense—an administrative decision inde-

    WHO SURVIVES ON DEATH ROW?—–617

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 617

    6 Because execution risk is small in the first 12

    years after a death sentence, we combine these years

    into one period and use it as the reference group. We

    then create three additional dummy variables: the

    first is coded 1 for periods since death sentence

    between 13 and 20 years, the second between 21 and

    23 years, and the third from year 24 on death row and

    later. This categorization captures the distribution of

    execution likelihoods as this distribution has a long

    flat tail to the left resulting from low initial risks and

    an inverted-U shaped distribuion thereafter. To dis-

    cover if this categorization is unrepresentative, we cre-

    ated alternative codes for death row years beyond 12.

    The significance test results and the theoretical impli-

    cations persist when we reestimate the models with

    these alternatives.

    7 This Web site is www.deathpenaltyinfo.org.

    Although the Supreme Court’s 1972 Furman decision

    made executions unconstitutional, Furman left open

    the possibility of a constitutional death penalty. Many

    death penalty states therefore maintained their death

    row populations and altered their statutes. In 1976 in

    Gregg and other cases the Court declared some mod-

    ified death penalty statutes constitutional (Paternoster

    1991; Zimring and Hawkins 1986). The data for a

    plausible study of death row outcomes before 1973

    apparently do not exist.8 Since 1973, 768 of the 820 offenders executed

    were matched. Data inconsistencies in these two

    sources explain the modest unmatched remainder.

    States analyzed are: Arizona, California, Delaware,

    Florida, Georgia, Illinois, Kansas, Kentucky,

    Maryland, Missouri, New Jersey, Ohio, Tennessee,

    Texas, Virginia, and Washington.

    Z

    A single

    line was

    permitted

    here to

    avoid

    excessive

    vertical

    justifica-

    tion; other-

    wise,

    everything

    from the A-

    head on

    would have

    been

    carried

    over.

    pendent of the state or offender characteristics

    at issue.9

    We therefore have information on victim race

    for all executed offenders and for about 28 per-

    cent of current or former death row offenders

    in these 16 states who were not executed.

    Although the execution rate from our sample is

    much higher than the equivalent rate in the

    CPUS data, such disparate rates should not bias

    our multivariate results. Our goal is to discov-

    er which explanatory variables affect execution

    rates. We are interested in how relative execu-

    tion risks shift based on, for example, the mur-

    der of a white instead of a nonwhite. Because

    we include all executed offenders in these states,

    but only a fraction of death row offenders who

    were not executed, our sample design is equiv-

    alent to a response-based sample design. Such

    a design uses all events that occur in a specified

    time period, but samples individuals who are at

    risk of experiencing the event but did not

    (Prentice and Pyke 1979). To analyze a

    response-based sample, Xie and Manski (1989)

    propose a weighted maximum likelihood esti-

    mator for a logit analysis. Xie and Manski

    demonstrate that estimates from a response-

    based sample behave asymptotically with no

    bias and with little loss in efficiency. We apply

    this weighted maximum likelihood estimator

    in our logit model. Following Xie and Manski

    (1989), we define a weight variable that equals

    qpwt =

    fp(3)

    where if p = 1, qp is the proportion executed

    from the CPUS sample and fp is the propor-

    tion executed from our sample; if p = 0, qp is

    the proportion not executed in the CPUS sam-

    ple and fp is the proportion not executed in our

    sample.10

    The selection procedures we use are like-

    ly to produce a random sample because our

    selection method depends on between data

    source matches. This assumption that our esti-

    mates of explanatory effects will be unbiased

    is corroborated by the results in Table 1 show-

    ing between sample contrasts. Most between

    sample offender attributes are extremely sim-

    ilar. Although Hispanics are underrepresent-

    ed and whites are overrepresented in our

    sample, this bias is eliminated by including

    offender race and ethnicity in all analyses.

    Table 1 shows a total of 4,145 (3,597 + 548)

    death row offenders between 1973 and 2002

    in 16 states from the CPUS data, and 13.2 per-

    cent were executed (548/4,145). In compari-

    son to their proportion in the population,

    disproportionately more African Americans

    and Hispanics were on death row. Of those

    executed, slightly more were white. An over-

    whelming majority of death row offenders

    were male (98 percent); and even higher pro-

    portions of males were executed (99.3 per-

    cent). Absent controls, death row offenders

    with prior conviction records faced a higher

    chance of execution than those with no prior

    conviction. Among current or former death

    row offenders, two-thirds killed a white, but

    among the executed offenders, four-f ifths

    killed a white. The multivariate analyses will

    show if victim race interacts with race of

    offender to alter execution likelihoods with

    other relevant factors held constant.

    Finally, because the proportion of Asian

    and Native American death row offenders is

    too modest to produce statistically signif i-

    cant results, these offenders are excluded

    618—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 618

    9 Included states exhibit considerable variation in

    execution probabilities. Most but not all of the miss-

    ing death penalty states are small with few at risk of

    execution. Two exceptions are North Carolina and

    Pennsylvania, but the corrections departments in

    these and the other missing states with death row pop-

    ulations would or could not provide offense dates, so

    the effects of victim race are not analyzed in these

    states. The funds from a generous grant were exhaust-

    ed by the matching process we had to use to obtain

    data on victim race. We therefore cannot employ

    costly supplemental data sources such as newspaper

    accounts, local prosecutor records (if any exist), or

    make detailed appraisal of this process in particular

    death penalty states.

    10 Weighted maximum likelihood estimates of a

    logit model make response-based samples behave

    asymptotically as if they were random (Manski and

    Lerman 1977). This method does not require that

    researchers select the best estimator. Even if the best

    estimator is probit rather than logit, Xie and Manski

    (1989) use Monte Carlo simulations to show that

    the weighted maximum likelihood logit estimator

    gives good results as long as response-based samples

    are larger than 1,000 as ours is.

    from our analyses. We also exclude offenders

    who died on death row for reasons other than

    an execution, but these decisions have no dis-

    cernable effects on the results.11 We therefore

    analyze outcomes experienced by 1,560 post-

    Furman death row offenders in 16 states from

    1973 to 2002. The corrections we use—that

    are grounded in the econometric literature on

    how such samples can be analyzed to pro-

    vide accurate f indings—should give unbi-

    a s e d a n d c o n s i s t e n t e s t i m a t e s o f t h e

    determinants of executions. This is so even

    though our sample includes all offenders who

    were executed, but only about 28 percent of

    those who were not executed in these 16

    states.

    MODEL SPECIFICATION AND EXPLANATORY

    VARIABLE MEASUREMENT

    One general specification of the discrete time

    logit model that predicts the log of execution

    odds for death row offender i in state j at time

    t is:

    Log EXECUTION ODDSij(t) =

    (td DURATION DUMMIES) +

    b1BLACK ij + b2HISPANICij +

    b3VWHITEij + b4(BLACK ij 3

    VWHITE ij) + b5(HISPANICij 3

    VWHITE ij) + b6MALEij +

    b7PRIORij + b8%BLKj(t–1) +

    b8%BLK2j(t–1) + (4)

    b10%HISP3j(t–1) + b11POPj(t–1) +

    b12MURDRTj(t–1) + b13BORNINSTj(t–1) +

    b14IDEOLOGYj(t–1) + b15%PPRSVOTEj(t–1) +

    b16 RLMEDINCj(t–1) + b17 %UNEMPj(t–1) +

    b18 %UNEMP2j(t–1) + b19YEAR92PLUSj +

    b20STHj

    where all explanatory variables are def ined

    below.12

    WHO SURVIVES ON DEATH ROW?—–619

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 619

    Table 1. Percentage Distributions of Offender Characteristics in 16 States by Samplea

    Sample in Which RaceTotal CPUS Sample of Victim is Available

    Not Executed Executed Not Executed Executed

    3,597 548 1,012 548

    Race (Percent)

    —White 48.1 55.5 53.6 55.5

    —Black 39.8 34.9 40.1 34.9

    —Hispanic 12.1 09.7 06.2 09.7

    Sex

    —Percent Maleb 98.3 99.3 98.0 99.3

    Marital Status at Offense

    —Percent Marriedb 24.0 30.8 21.2 30.8

    College Education at Offense

    —Percent Yesb 07.6 07.7 10.0 07.7

    Prior Convictions

    —Percent Yesb 59.4 68.2 59.5 68.2

    Race of the Victim (Percent)

    —White 66.5 80.3

    —Nonwhite 33.5 19.7

    a Marital status and college education at offense cannot be used in the logit regressions because too many missing

    values are present; we provide their distributions to better compare samples that do not include and include race

    of victim.b These percentages plus the omitted category for each variable sum to 100 percent.

    11 We eliminated 179 offenders from the sample

    because they died on death row before execution; 118

    Native Americans and 34 Asians or Pacific Islanders

    also were eliminated leaving a total of 1,560 offend-

    ers. In analyses (not shown) we find that these exclu-

    sions do not affect the reported results. Deficiencies

    in the data made efforts to code race of victim in the

    few instances when there were multiple victims too

    speculative, so most of these cases were omitted.

    12 We use exhaustive models. According to

    Johnston (1984), “It is more serious to omit relevant

    INDIVIDUAL EFFECTS. We create two dummy

    variables for minority offenders: the first equals

    1 if an offender is African American (BLACK),

    the second equals 1 if an offender is Hispanic

    (HISPANIC), and both dummies are set equal

    to 0 if the condition in question is not true.

    Another dummy variable is coded 1 only if the

    victim is white (VWHITE). We next create two

    interaction ter ms: the f irst (BLACK 3

    VWHITE) is equal to the dummy variable coded

    1 for an African American offender times the

    dummy variable coded 1 for a white victim; the

    second (HISPANIC 3 VWHITE) is equal to the

    dummy variable coded 1 for an Hispanic offend-

    er times the dummy variable coded 1 for a white

    victim. Additional individual effects are held

    constant with a dummy variable coded 1 if a

    death row inmate is male (MALE) and anoth-

    er coded 1 if a death row inmate has prior con-

    victions (PRIOR).13

    CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS. We measure racial

    threat effects with the percentage of African

    Americans (%BLK) and the percentage of

    Hispanic residents (%HISP) in each state, but

    in contrast to all other contextual variables, the

    percentage of Hispanics in the states is unavail-

    able in the non-census years before 1990. We

    therefore use decennial census figures in the five

    years after a census and figures from the next

    census in the next five years before 1990; after

    1990 this variable is time varying by year (the

    results persist if we use yearly linear interpola-

    tions to estimate these missing values).14 To

    capture nonlinear quadratic relationships, both

    minority threat variables are entered in untrans-

    formed and squared form. All state-level vari-

    ables, save the percentage of Republican votes

    for president and Hispanics (which time vary by

    four or five years), are time-varying by year.

    Following convention in studies of public pol-

    icy, the yearly time-varying explanatory vari-

    ables are lagged by a year because their effects

    should not be instantaneous.

    Theory, however, suggests that Hispanic pres-

    ence should have a nonlinear relationship with

    execution probabilities that becomes increas-

    ingly stronger as this indicator reaches extreme

    values, yet there is no reason to think this asso-

    ciation should shift direction. Higher percent-

    ages of Hispanics, for example, may well

    produce increasingly greater execution proba-

    bilities, but it is difficult to see how growth

    from a low to a somewhat higher percentage of

    Hispanics could lead to reductions in execu-

    tion probabilities. When a priori considerations

    suggest that such nonlinear relationships that

    should not change direction are present, power

    transformations are most appropriate (Cohen et

    al. 2003:225–54). Threat theory thus suggests

    that a single exponential transformation will be

    best. To avoid iterative searches for the best

    exponent and overfitting, we cube the percent-

    age of Hispanics. This power transformation is

    somewhat unconventional in sociology (but not

    in psychology). To provide comparisons and

    reassure the reader, we present otherwise iden-

    tical models with and without this transforma-

    tion.

    We assess the influence of the division of

    labor with population (POP). Following Jacobs

    and Carmichael (2002), who find that this meas-

    620—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 620

    variables than to include irrelevant variables since in

    the former case the coefficients will be biased, the dis-

    turbance variance overestimated, and conventional

    inference procedures rendered invalid, while in the

    latter case the coefficients will be unbiased, the dis-

    turbance variance properly estimated, and the infer-

    ence procedures properly estimated. This constitutes

    a fairly strong case for including rather than exclud-

    ing relevant variables in equations. There is, howev-

    er, a qualification. Adding extra variables, be they

    relevant or irrelevant, will lower the precision of esti-

    mation of the relevant coefficients” (p. 262). Hence

    our comprehensive models should produce more

    accurate point estimates but the significance tests will

    be relatively conservative.13 Although we cannot introduce a control for

    offense severity because information is not avail-

    able, a legal requirement probably makes such a con-

    trol unnecessary. In the Gregg case that relegalized

    capital punishment, the Court held that jurors can sen-

    tence offenders to death only if they deem the crime

    to be sufficiently horrific. Aggravating criteria stip-

    ulated by legislatures that jurors must consider when

    making this judgment include the killing of multiple

    victims, murdering a child, or homicides that involve

    deliberate torture (Paternoster 1991). Such prereq-

    uisites probably create a more precise control for

    offense severity than the alternatives used in studies

    that assess sentencing for non-capital crimes.

    14 The great majority of the death row outcomes

    we study occurred after 1990 when the percentage of

    Hispanics in the states is calculated with yearly data.

    ure of outsider presence explains whether a

    state has a legal death penalty, outsiders are

    measured with a dummy coded 1 if over 75

    percent of a state’s population was born in-state

    (BORNINST). We assess partisanship with the

    percentage of a state’s vote for the Republican

    candidate in the last presidential election

    (PPRSVOTE). The threat from higher murder

    rates is gauged by murder rates provided by the

    Uniform Crime Reports.

    Berry and colleagues (1998) view mass ide-

    ologies (IDEOLOGY) as the mean on a liber-

    al-conservative continuum. They identify the

    ideological position of each member of

    Cong ress using interest-g roup ratings

    (Americans for Democratic Action, Committee

    on Political Education) of a representative’s vot-

    ing record and then estimate mass ideology in

    each congressional district with this score for the

    district’s incumbent and with an estimated score

    for the incumbent’s challenger in the last elec-

    tion. Incumbent ideology scores are combined

    with estimated challenger ideology scores

    weighted by district election results to capture

    district ideologies. Berry and colleagues cal-

    culate state-level scores on liberalism-conser-

    vatism with the mean of these within-state

    congressional district scores. We analyze the

    most recent version of this index, which gauges

    congressional votes until 2003 and adds a few

    corrections to the values first published in 1998.

    The most liberal states receive the highest

    scores, so the coefficients on this variable should

    be negative.15

    We measure the tax base and a state’s abili-

    ty to support this costly punishment with real

    median household income (RLMEDINC). We

    capture joblessness with the percentage of

    unemployed (%UNEMP). Because this factor

    may have a nonlinear relationship, we include

    its square. We also include a dummy variable

    coded 1 (YEAR92PLUS) for years after 1992

    when execution frequencies increased and a

    dummy variable coded 1 for southern states

    (STH). We use one-tailed tests because theo-

    retically based predictions about sign have been

    stipulated.16

    ANALYSES

    DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS AND

    MULTIVARIATE MODELS OF EXECUTION

    PROBABILITIES

    INITIAL ANALYSES. Figure 1 shows the distribu-

    tion of death row outcomes during the years in

    question and Table 2 shows the predicted signs,

    means, and standard deviations. In Table 3 we

    begin the multivariate analyses by restricting the

    first model to individual explanatory factors to

    show contrasts with subsequent models that

    include contextual effects. Recall that two

    dummy variables are set equal to 1 if offenders

    are African American or Hispanic. A third is set

    equal to 1 if the victim is white, and this victim

    variable is interacted with the two minority

    offender variables to create two interaction

    terms. The coefficients on these two product

    terms capture what happens to either African

    American or Hispanic offenders who killed

    whites (all necessary main effects are included).

    A sixth dummy variable is set equal to 1 if a con-

    demned offender is male, while the last is equal

    to 1 if an offender has prior convictions. In

    Model 2 we begin to add contextual variables

    by entering the percentage of African American

    and Hispanic residents in quadratic form. To

    assess division of labor effects, population is

    included as well. We add the percentage born in-

    WHO SURVIVES ON DEATH ROW?—–621

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 621

    15 This index has face validity. From 1972 to 2002

    Rhode Island and Massachusetts tied for second as

    the most liberal states. But the sole Vermont repre-

    sentative—who probably was the only member of

    Congress who claimed to be a socialist in this peri-

    od—earned the highest liberalism score. Mississippi

    in 1972 had the most conservative score followed by

    Virginia in 1974. Specialists have accepted the use

    of roll-call, vote-based indexes to identify ideology

    (Fowler 1982; Poole and Rosenthal 2000). Rankings

    by Americans for Democratic Action (ADA) and the

    AFL-CIO’s Committee on Political Education

    (COPE) are the most widely used and have withstood

    considerable scrutiny (Herrera, Epperlein, and Smith

    1995; Shaffer 1989).

    16 Despite repeated attempts we have not located

    information on the partisanship of state appellate

    court justices. In almost all states, one appellate court

    handles each level of state death row appeals, so

    variation in offender outcomes probably cannot be

    attributed to within state appellate court differences.

    Data on factors such as offender demeanor, their

    behavior in prison, offense characteristics other than

    those measured, and additional information on

    offender appeals unfortunately are not available.

    622—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 622

    Figure 1. Three Death Row Outcomes in 16 States by Years After Death Sentence

    Table 2. Predicted Signs, Means, and Standard Deviations

    Variable Predicted Sign Mean SD

    1 if Executed .010 .099

    1 if Black Offender + .390 .488

    1 if Hispanic Offender + .068 .251

    1 if White Victim + .710 .454

    1 if Black 3 1 if White Victim + .161 .367

    1 if Hispanic 3 1 if White Victim + .036 .1871 if Male + .986 .116

    1 if Prior Conviction + .650 .477

    Percent Black + 12.772 5.331

    Percent Black2 – 191.548 171.170

    Percent Hispanic 0 15.212 11.189

    Percent Hispanic3/10,000 + .943 1.169

    Population/1,000,000 – 13.751 8.021

    1 if Percent Born in State > 75 Percent – .225 .417

    Murder Rate per 100,000 + 9.332 2.906

    Citizen Ideology – 43.608 9.149

    Percent Republican Vote for President + 48.695 9.330

    Real Median Household Income/100 + 366.480 47.638

    Percent Unemployed + 6.051 1.532

    1 if Southern State + .528 .499

    Note: Weighted by 16,361 offender-years from 16 sampled states.

    state and a quadratic test of the effects of unem-

    ployment in Model 3.

    The results in Model 1 show that several indi-

    vidual offender attributes contribute to execu-

    tion probabilities. The coefficient on the first

    main effect shows that African American offend-

    ers whose victims are not white are less likely

    to be executed. But the coefficients on the two

    interaction terms suggest that African Americans

    or Hispanics found guilty of killing members of

    the majority race face a greater likelihood of this

    punishment (we defer discussing significance

    tests on contrasts between the coefficients on

    individual variables until discussion of the best

    model). If they persist, these last findings are

    particularly important as they suggest that the

    extra-legal attribute with the most powerful

    effects on death sentences accounts for post-

    death sentence execution probabilities as well.

    In Model 2 we add five contextual variables

    to the individual determinants in the first model.

    Although these additions increase model

    explanatory power as the BIC statistic falls

    sharply, the coefficients on the interaction terms

    that assess execution probabilities for minorities

    who kill whites remain significant. We again

    find that African Americans convicted of mur-

    dering nonwhites are less likely to be executed,

    but execution probabilities remain higher for

    African Americans who kill a white. The con-

    textual results show that execution probabilities

    are g reater in states with more African

    Americans until a threshold in this proportion

    is reached (we also defer reporting this inflec-

    tion point until the presentation of the best

    model). Although the percentage of Hispanic

    residents has no effect, this ethnic threat vari-

    able in squared form enhances execution prob-

    abilities. Additional findings show that states

    with larger populations and a greater division of

    labor are not as likely to execute. After we add

    the unemployment rate and its square together

    with the percentage born in-state, these initial

    results suggest that unemployment rates influ-

    ence execution probabilities, but this penalty is

    less likely in states with few outsiders.

    COMPREHENSIVE MODELS. Yet other accounts

    may matter. In Model 4 of Table 4, we add state

    murder rates, citizen ideology, votes for

    Republican presidential candidates, real medi-

    an income, and a dummy variable for years

    after 1992 when the number of executions

    expanded. Model 5 is identical to Model 4, but

    we replace the quadratic Hispanic threat spec-

    ification with the cubic power transformation.

    Model 6 only differs from Model 5 because a

    WHO SURVIVES ON DEATH ROW?—–623

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 623

    Table 3. Post-Capital Sentencing Determinants of Executions Estimated with Discrete-Time Logit

    Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

    Explanatory Variables Coef. SE Coef. SE Coef. SE

    Individual Effects

    —1 if Black Offender –1.015*** .349 –.761*** .220 –.758*** .215

    —1 if Hispanic Offender –.375 .465 .131 .228 .102 .238

    —1 if White Victim –.348 .258 –.067 .230 –.157 .219

    —1 if Black Off. 3 1 if White Victim 1.394*** .371 .898** .322 1.042*** .337

    —1 if Hispanic Off. 3 1 if White Victim 1.130** .422 .332 .255 .449* .257—1 if Male .458 .499 .907* .545 .870 .539

    —1 if Prior Conviction .082 .130 .134 .103 .123 .086

    Contextual Effects

    —Percent Black .— .— .443*** .106 .423*** .077

    —Percent Black2 .— .— –.011*** .003 –.012*** .002

    —Percent Hispanic .— .— –.052 .058 –.036 .054

    —Percent Hispanic2 .— .— –.005*** .001 .003** .001

    —Population/1,000,000 .— .— –.149*** .040 –.100*** .029

    —1 if Percent Born in State > 75 Percent .— .— .— .— –1.813*** .322

    —Percent Unemployed .— .— .— .— –1.144** .392

    —Percent Unemployed2 .— .— .— .— .069** .027

    Constant –5.101*** .633 –8.556*** 1.118 4.093*** 1.258

    Log Likelihood –873.3*** –816.6*** –793.7***

    BIC Statistic 1853.4 1778.8 1732.8

    Notes: N = 1,560 offenders and 16,361 offender-years from 16 states; state-level cluster corrected standard

    errors; coefficients on offender-year dummies not shown.

    * p ≤ .05; ** p ≤ .01; *** p ≤ .001 (one-tailed tests except for intercept).

    control for state location in the South is added

    to the explanatory variables entered in Model 5.

    The results in Model 4 show that the addition

    of political variables and the tax base measure

    increase model explanatory power as the BIC

    statistic again declines from its value in Model

    3. The findings confirm other contextual expec-

    tations: liberal states are less likely to execute,

    but capital punishment likelihoods are higher in

    states where Republican presidential candidates

    received the most votes and where the tax base

    is substantial. These findings show that the mur-

    der rates have the expected positive relationship

    with executions, but the unemployment rates are

    no longer significant after the addition of con-

    textual variables that assess state political envi-

    ronments. When we replace the quadratic

    Hispanic specification with the cubic power

    alternative in Model 5, all results persist, but the

    coefficient on Hispanic threat becomes signif-

    icant.

    A test of the significance of the difference

    between the absolute value of the coefficient on

    the black offender main effect and the coeffi-

    cient on the interaction term that gauges exe-

    cution likelihoods for blacks who kill whites

    shows that the interaction term coefficient is

    greater than its counterpart on the main effect

    (two-tailed p = .038). Other tests that gauge the

    statistical significance of contrasts reveal that

    the coefficient on the blacks who kill whites

    term is most substantial. For example, when

    we replace the condemned blacks main effect

    with the same variable for whites, we find iden-

    tical significance test results again indicating

    that the coeff icient on the black killings of

    whites interaction term is more substantial.

    Similar tests show that the coefficient on the

    624—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 624

    Table 4. Post-Capital Sentencing Determinants of Executions Estimated with Discrete-Time Logit

    Model 4 Model 5 Model 6

    Explanatory Variables Coef. SE Coef. SE Coef. SE

    Individual Effects

    —1 if Black Offender –.737*** .233 –.744*** .234 –.764*** .231

    —1 if Hispanic Offender .108 .248 .107 .248 .099 .253

    —1 if White Victim –.141 .202 –.144 .202 –.158 .200

    —1 if Black Off. 3 1 if White Victim 1.007** .339 1.011** .342 1.033*** .334

    —1 if Hispanic Off. 3 1 if White Victim .429* .256 .437* .263 .454* .269—1 if Male .845 .527 .852 .531 .848 .530

    —1 if Prior Conviction .118* .069 .119* .073 .121 .075

    Contextual Effects

    —Percent Black .450*** .066 .439*** .065 .479*** .113

    —Percent Black2 –.014*** .002 –.014*** .002 –.014*** .003

    —Percent Hispanic –.019 .068 .— .— .— .—

    —Percent Hispanic2 .002 .002 .— .— .— .—

    —Percent Hispanic3/10,000 .— .— .533*** .109 .569*** .134

    —Population/1,000,000 –.099*** .025 –.099*** .027 –.099*** .026

    —1 if Percent Born in State > 75 Percent –2.000*** .265 –1.998*** .284 –2.089*** .397

    —Murder Rate .088* .045 .102** .043 .102* .045

    —Citizen Ideology –.030** .011 –.029** .011 –.033*** .009

    —Percent Republican Vote for President .048*** .011 .048*** .012 .052*** .010

    —Real Median Household Income /100 .010*** .003 .010*** .003 .009** .003

    —Percent Unemployed –.749 .463 –.778 .493 –.783 .500

    —Percent Unemployed2 .050 .033 .052 .035 .052 .036

    —1 if after 1992 1.597*** .307 1.602*** .316 1.635*** .302

    —1 if Southern State .— .— .— .— –.291 .611

    Constant –12.123*** 2.268 –12.016*** 1.951 –12.004*** 1.966

    Log Likelihood –776.1*** –775.6*** –775.5***

    BIC Statistic 1697.7 1696.8 1696.5

    Notes: N = 1,560 offenders and 16,361 offender-years from 16 states; state-level cluster corrected standard

    errors; coefficients on offender-year dummies not shown.

    * p ≤ .05; ** p ≤ .01; *** p ≤ .001 (one-tailed tests except for intercept).

    black killings of whites interaction term is

    greater than the coefficient on the Hispanic-

    white victim interaction term (two-tailed p =

    .023). Finally, when we add the South in the last

    model, we find no noteworthy changes in the

    results.

    We now can calculate the inflection point

    where the relationship between the percentage

    of African Americans and execution probabili-

    ties shifts from positive to negative. Again using

    coefficients from the best model (Model 5), we

    find that this change occurs after the percent-

    age of African American residents reaches 16.2

    or about the 86th percentile in the percentage of

    African American residents in these 16 states in

    this period. This result suggests that execution

    probabilities start to fall only after the potential

    African American vote reaches a rather sub-

    stantial threshold. And odds ratios suggest that

    these effects are not weak. The exponentiated

    coefficient from Model 5 that gauges execution

    probabilities for African Americans convicted

    of killing a white is 2.75. The odds ratio for

    African Americans who kill nonwhites is .476.

    The odds ratio for Hispanics convicted of killing

    a white is 1.55. The size of some contextual

    effects are substantial as well: for example, the

    odds ratio for the percentage of blacks in these

    states is 1.55 and its counterpart on the cube of

    Hispanic presence is 1.70.

    ADDITIONAL CONSIDERATIONS. Some death

    row inmates accept their sentences and try to

    stop all appeals. These “volunteers” are dis-

    proportionately white (Lofquist 2002). In states

    that rarely execute, a greater proportion of the

    few executed offenders are volunteers (Lofquist

    2002), so a failure to control for this effect may

    bias the estimates. The data at hand do not let

    us identify such offenders, but we can discov-

    er if this effect matters because volunteers are

    executed with less delay. If those executed early

    differ sharply from those executed later, analy-

    ses limited to inmates executed later—or mod-

    els restricted to offenders executed from 5 to 10

    years after sentencing—should produce findings

    that differ, but they do not. The same variables

    are significant in each of these five restricted

    analyses (not shown but available on request).

    Such theoretically identical findings suggest

    that our inability to directly control for volun-

    teers has not distorted the findings.

    We find no evidence for additional interac-

    tion effects and these negative results persist

    when we assess cross-level interactions between

    individual and jurisdictional characteristics.

    This result is important for methodological rea-

    sons. Cross-level interaction significance tests

    are too optimistic if such tests are not estimat-

    ed with an Hierarchical Linear Model (HLM)

    approach. Yet none of these interactions are sig-

    nificant when they are assessed with the non-

    HLM method we employ. Such cross-level

    effects therefore will not be uncovered if they

    are estimated with HLM because this estimator

    will produce larger estimates of the standard

    errors. The cluster adjustment we use to correct

    the standard errors for within state interdepen-

    dencies therefore should suffice, as HLM will

    produce the same nonsignificant and redun-

    dant findings about cross-level interactions.17

    We entered religious fundamentalism along

    with various measures of Republican gover-

    nors and this party’s strength in state legislatures,

    but these determinants had no effects (models

    not shown). We find no evidence that federal

    judge partisanship influences executions.18 To

    isolate the effects of otherwise omitted cross-

    state national political, social, or macroeco-

    nomic changes, we included dummies coded for

    WHO SURVIVES ON DEATH ROW?—–625

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 625

    17 For readers who prefer two-tailed significance

    tests even if theoretical justification for direction is

    provided, we list coefficients that reached the one- but

    not the two-tailed .05 level: in Table 3, the male

    offender variable in Model 2 is significant at the .05

    one- but not the two-tailed threshold and the same is

    true for the Hispanics who killed whites interaction

    term in Model 3. In Table 4, each of the coefficients

    on the Hispanics convicted of killing a white inter-

    action term are significant at the one- but not the two-

    tailed level and the same generality holds for the

    coeff icient on the prior conviction variable in

    Model 4.18 We gauged the degree to which the estimates are

    robust by dropping each of the 16 sampled states in

    analyses not shown. The implications persist but this

    dependent variable already was skewed. To avoid a

    logit model that could not be estimated after we

    removed a state with many executions and increased

    dependent variable skewness, we had to remove a few

    explanatory variables from some of these 16 equa-

    tions. Only one of the 16 sampled death penalty

    states (Kansas, which had a tiny death row popula-

    tion) had no executions. The elimination of states with

    few executions had only tiny effects on the estimates.

    each year (models not shown). All explanatory

    variables that matter in Table 4 were significant

    in these models. Such results suggest that shifts

    in the national political or macroeconomic cli-

    mate cannot account for these results. The mod-

    els in Table 4 pass the link test for

    misspecification. These considerations provide

    added reasons to think that the most compre-

    hensive models capture the primary determi-

    nants of death row outcomes.19

    We nevertheless must acknowledge that we

    were forced to use advanced statistical tech-

    niques to overcome data limitations. Although

    there are good reasons to believe that the report-

    ed estimates are unbiased and consistent, supe-

    rior data always are preferable to such statistical

    alternatives (for a forceful illustration, see

    Donohue and Wolfors 2005). In particular, we

    hope that subsequent researchers can obtain

    more exhaustive information on victim race

    and other considerations from additional death

    penalty states. Perhaps researchers with the

    resources and the time to achieve a better rap-

    port with state correction agency officials who

    would not cooperate with our requests for data

    can produce superior findings about this issue

    by analyzing additional states.

    DISCUSSION

    THE FINDINGS

    Possibly because offender prior convictions

    contribute to prosecutors’ decisions to seek the

    death sentence and lead trial courts to support

    this request, we find weak and inconsistent evi-

    dence that this factor affects post-death sen-

    tence execution probabilities. The findings show

    that gender is nonsignificant, but the proportion

    of condemned women in our sample is tiny. Yet

    victim race clearly is the most important indi-

    vidual finding. The coefficients and the signif-

    icance tests that gauge contrasts in the size of

    these point estimates show that blacks convict-

    ed of killing whites are more likely to be exe-

    cuted than other death row offenders. Such

    results do not contradict hypotheses that local

    prosecutors with an interest in protecting well-

    publicized legal victories that should further

    their political careers make successful efforts to

    resist death row appeals. These findings also

    support a hypothesis that state justices do not

    ignore pressures to deny appellate relief to

    minority death row offenders who kill whites.

    But the findings suggest that African Americans

    on death row for killing nonwhites are less like-

    ly to be executed than other condemned pris-

    oners.

    The evidence corroborates hypotheses that

    Hispanics also face higher execution probabil-

    ities if their victims are white. Yet the odds

    ratios that gauge the strength of this Hispanic

    effect and the untransformed coefficients are

    significantly weaker than their counterparts that

    assess the strength of the black offender, white

    victim effect. Such contrasting results about

    the explanatory power of ethnic rather than

    racial effects should not be surprising in light

    of the fiercely divisive and violent conflicts

    about race throughout U.S. history (Myrdal

    1944; Tocqueville 1948). Only a few states had

    substantial Hispanic populations before the

    1990s. This ethnic group may have been too

    small in most death penalty states to be suffi-

    ciently threatening. Subsequent studies, how-

    ever, may provide greater support for this ethnic

    threat account because the Hispanic population

    recently has expanded so rapidly.

    The contextual results always show that

    minority proportions matter. Larger percent-

    ages of African American residents produce

    higher execution probabilities, but this effect

    becomes negative only after the proportion of

    African Americans reaches a relatively sub-

    stantial threshold. The positive sign on the

    unsquared coefficient provides evidence for a

    racial threat account, but the negative sign on

    the squared percentage of African American

    residents suggests that execution probabilities

    respond to election pressures. The form of the

    nonlinear relationship between Hispanic pro-

    portions in a state and execution probabilities

    626—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 626

    19 Attempts to use other diagnostic tests failed

    because these estimates are weighted. Tests con-

    ducted without the weights suggest that estimation

    problems are not present. No observation has a high

    leverage score, and there are only a few modest out-

    liers. Estimate stability and a VIF analysis conduct-

    ed on the state variables (without the squared terms)

    yields a maximum score 4.33. Both considerations

    suggest that collinearity is not present. Although we

    analyze 16 states, there are 480 state-years as all but

    two of our state-level variables are time-varying by

    year. Fewer case-years are common in pooled time-

    series analysis.

    differs from the nonlinear association between

    African American presence and these proba-

    bilities. Probably because the proportion of

    Hispanics in all but a few states was so modest,

    the findings indicate that a growth in the per-

    centage of Hispanics yields increasingly

    stronger execution probabilities without a rever-

    sal in the sign of this relationship.

    Consistent with prior findings on the factors

    that produce a legal death penalty in the states

    (Jacobs and Carmichael 2002), the findings in

    this article suggest that jurisdictions with the

    most residents born in-state are less likely to use

    this ultimate punishment. This association prob-

    ably is based on latent hostility to strangers

    (Hale 1996) and to a corresponding reluctance

    to invoke the ultimate penalty against people

    who are regarded as one’s neighbors. A differ-

    ent aggregate finding supports another threat

    account, as the results show that execution prob-

    abilities are greater in states that have the high-

    est murder rates.

    Votes for Republican presidential candidates,

    who often run on law and order platforms, help

    explain execution likelihoods as well. Beckett’s

    (1997) findings suggest that law and order cam-

    paign rhetoric magnifies the public salience of

    the crime issue. Successful law and order polit-

    ical campaigns therefore should produce greater

    support for the harshest punishment. The sen-

    sitizing effects of this rhetoric and the relative

    success of Republican law and order candidates

    in some jurisdictions—which almost certainly

    indicates considerable preexisting support for

    capital punishment—help explain why votes

    for Republican candidates provide such a robust

    explanation for executions. It is interesting that

    the presence of a Republican governor does not

    increase execution probabilities (results not

    shown), but this finding should not be surpris-

    ing as this factor does not explain the legaliza-

    tion of capital punishment (Jacobs and

    Carmichael 2002). Governors must decide the

    final appeal before an execution. This awesome

    moral responsibility gives these political offi-

    cials good reason to be ambivalent about this

    punishment (Jacobs and Carmichael 2002;

    Zimring and Hawkins 1986).

    As theorists such as Garland (1990, 2001) and

    Savelsberg (1994) would expect, the explanatory

    power of political ideology suggests that polit-

    ical values contribute to the propensity to exe-

    cute. When or where there is greater political

    support for liberal values, execution probabili-

    ties diminish. This result supports claims

    (Lakoff 1996) that support for the death penal-

    ty is one of the most important differences

    between liberals and conservatives. Finally, the

    results suggest that the affluent states that can

    better afford this costly punishment use it more

    often.

    WIDER IMPLICATIONS

    This analysis fills critical gaps in the sparse lit-

    erature on the application of capital punish-

    ment. First, we gauged the effects of

    offender-victim racial contrasts and found that

    victim race is a strong determinant of execu-

    tions. Until this study, and despite the empiri-

    cal strength of victim race in trial-court studies

    about the death sentence process, apparently

    no research has gauged the explanatory power

    of this factor. In addition to the findings about

    theoretically important individual and legal fac-

    tors, our data set lets us test environmental

    determinants that have been ignored in the lit-

    erature. The findings suggest that these con-

    textual omissions are unfor tunate, as

    environmental accounts have substantial

    explanatory power in this and in other analyses

    that seek to explain criminal justice outcomes.

    Compared to the studies of the determinants of

    death sentences that are restricted to individual

    data from one or a few jurisdictions, this study’s

    coverage is far greater as we assessed the effects

    of both individual and contextual accounts over

    a 30-year period in a diverse set of multiple

    states.

    Reviews of the related research on race and

    felony sentencing (Chiricos and Crawford 1995;

    Walker et al. 1996) suggest that substantial dis-

    agreement exists in this literature. These reviews

    both base their conclusions on results that appear

    in only a bare majority of the many available

    investigations. Almost all of the many studies

    that analyze trial court sentencing are based on

    offender samples from just one or only a few

    trial courts in a specific region. Yet since trial

    courts exist in diverse environments, inconsis-

    tent results can be expected. If each court study

    investigates death penalty sentences in just one

    or a few localities, and their political climates

    differ, it will be difficult to uncover general

    relationships, particularly because court envi-

    ronments have so much explanatory power

    WHO SURVIVES ON DEATH ROW?—–627

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 627

    (Helms and Jacobs 2002). The same reasoning

    holds for death penalty investigations. It follows

    that a study of executions in many states that

    assesses both contextual and individual factors

    should provide more accurate results than would

    analyses restricted to individual explanations

    in just one or only a few jurisdictions.

    Another plausible explanation for these dis-

    agreements concerns the neglect of political

    effects in almost all of the studies of criminal

    justice system outcomes. As one might expect

    in light of the recent theoretical emphasis on the

    political nature of legal punishments (Chambliss

    1994; Foucault 1977; Garland 1990, 2001) and

    the Republican party’s tactical use of law and

    order appeals to covertly enhance voters’ racial

    fears (see the quotes in note 4), these results

    show that executions increased after states

    awarded additional votes to Republican candi-

    dates. Yet increased electoral support for law and

    order Republicans who enthusiastically embrace

    the death penalty may be based on preexisting

    conservative values. But this alternative hypoth-

    esis is unlikely as votes for Republican presi-

    dential candidates account for executions after

    citizen ideology has been held constant. Such

    findings, of course, show that these supposed-

    ly objective and purely legal decisions about

    who will die are shaped by factors that should

    not be relevant.

    These findings also confirm the racial poli-

    tics perspective that provided the primary con-

    ceptual impetus for this research. In the

    homogeneous European democracies, predom-

    inant penal emphasis is on the reintegration of

    offenders into a solidaristic society adminis-

    tered by experts who are only distantly account-

    able to the voters (Savelsberg 1994; Whitman

    2003). In the United States, however, the pub-

    lic’s Manichean image of human nature, com-

    bined with a history of bitter racial conflict,

    has produced an exclusionary penal system.

    According to Wacquant (2000, 2001), the

    resilient divisions produced by slavery and by

    the later virulent measures used to maintain

    white supremacy created a segregative penal

    solution consistent with the dominant public

    image of criminals as members of a vile racial

    underclass. This racial history and the resulting

    cultural premises helped to produce a criminal

    justice system that primarily uses incapacitation

    to control street crime. The most reliable way to

    incapacitate incorrigible offenders is to kill

    them. This incapacitative solution fits with the

    prevailing public view—often forcefully

    expressed politically in this most direct of all

    large democracies—that the primary goal of

    the U.S. criminal justice system should be

    vengeance.

    Executions therefore should be most likely in

    jurisdictions in which covertly racist law and

    order political appeals have been most suc-

    cessful and where the presence of a large African

    American population—that has not become

    quite large enough to enforce its political val-

    ues—threatens dominant whites. Prior research

    supports such political and racial results.

    Findings show that the same aggregate politi-

    cal and racial factors account for the likelihood

    that a state will legalize the death penalty

    (Jacobs and Carmichael 2002). In another study,

    Jacobs and colleagues (2005) report that simi-

    lar political and racial factors explain the num-

    ber of death sentences. But compared to these

    two studies and other aggregate-level research,

    this investigation is unique as it analyzes both

    state and individual characteristics. Probably

    the most important finding that emerges from

    this multilevel approach concerns victim race.

    African Americans who breach the racial caste

    system by killing whites can more often expect

    the harshest of all penalties, and this finding

    holds after many contextual level political and

    racial effects have been held constant. Such

    results do not contradict Wacquant’s (2000,

    2001) theoretical claims that a primary goal in

    the current U.S. criminal justice system is to sus-

    tain the prior racial caste system even though the

    current methods are less transparent than the

    lawless brutality used in the past.20

    The most important legal implication con-

    cerns racial equity. Many claims have been

    made that the U.S. criminal justice system is not

    colorblind, yet after multiple studies, definitive

    evidence showing that the trial courts sentence

    African Americans less leniently than whites has

    remained elusive. This study instead analyzed

    racial effects on the post-sentencing execution

    process. The findings show that despite efforts

    628—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 628

    20 For additional findings on how past racial, eth-

    nic, or religiously based brutality continues to affect

    current practices, see Archer and Gartner (1984),

    Messner, Baller, and Zevenbergen (2005), Savelsberg

    and King (2005), and Jacobs and colleagues (2005).

    to transcend an unfortunate racial past, residues

    of this fierce discrimination evidently still linger,

    at least when the most morally critical decision

    about punishment is decided. Findings indicat-

    ing that African Americans who kill nonwhites

    are less likely to be executed than their coun-

    terparts who kill whites show that the post-sen-

    tencing capital punishment process continues to

    place greater value on white lives. Evidently,

    inclinations to devalue African Americans in

    constitutional compromises in the distant past

    remain today, although they are expressed in less

    conspicuous ways. Associate Justice William

    Brennan helps us understand the importance

    of such discriminatory findings:

    Those whom we would banish from society or

    from the human community itself often speak in

    too faint a voice to be heard above society’s

    demand for punishment. It is the particular role of

    the courts to hear these voices, for the Constitution

    declares that the majoritarian chorus may not alone

    dictate the conditions of social life. (Brennan 1987)

    This evidence shows that the state and federal

    appellate courts—who should pay special atten-

    tion to those accused of the most horrif ic

    crimes—evidently continue to listen to some

    voices more than others.

    David Jacobs is Professor of Sociology and (by cour-

    tesy) Political Science at The Ohio State University.

    He uses a political economy approach to study out-

    comes in the criminal justice system and other issues

    in political sociology. A study of racial politics recent-

    ly appeared in the American Journal of Sociology

    while another publication on the determinants of

    yearly execution frequencies will soon appear in

    Social Problems. In addition to these interests, he con-

    tinues to investigate the politics of labor relations.

    Zhenchao Qian is Professor in the Department of

    Sociology and research associate in the Initiative in

    Population Research at The Ohio State University. His

    research focuses on family demography, race and

    ethnicity, and immigration. He studies changes in

    mate selection by taking into account marriage mar-

    ket conditions. His work with Daniel T. Lichter, recent-

    ly published in American Sociological Review,

    centers on changes in racial/ethnic intermarriage. His

    other research examines changing racial identifica-

    tion among children born to interracial couples.

    Jason T. Carmichael is an Assistant Professor in the

    Department of Sociology at McGill University. His

    interests include criminology, criminal justice, and,

    more broadly, political sociology. Current projects

    include, among other things: an analysis of the polit-

    ical and social determinants of the number of juve-

    nile delinquents who are adjudicated in the adult

    criminal justice system, and an examination of the

    ability of foundation funding to shape the size and/or

    success of social movement organizations.

    Stephanie L. Kent is an Assistant Professor of

    Sociology at Cleveland State University. Her research

    focuses on the politics of crime control and uses

    macrosocial explanations to predict social control

    outcomes. Recent publications include a pooled-

    time-series analysis of police strength in U.S. cities

    published in Criminology. Additional work on capi-

    tal punishment has or will be published in the

    American Sociological Review and Social Problems.

    She is currently exploring the social and political

    determinants of homicide and the use of lethal force

    by and against the police in U.S. cities.

    REFERENCES

    Aarons, Dwight. 1998. “Getting Out of this Mess:

    Steps Toward Addressing and Avoiding Inordinate

    Delay in Capital Cases.” Journal of Criminal Law

    and Criminology 89:1–58.

    Allen, Michael P. and John L. Campbell. 1994. “State

    Revenue Extraction from Different Income

    Groups.” American Sociological Review

    59:169–86.

    Archer, Dane and Rosemary Gartner. 1984. Violence

    and Crime in Cross-National Perspective. New

    Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Bailey, William C. and Ruth Peterson. 1999. “Capital

    Punishment, Homicide, and Deterrence.” Pp.

    257–77 in Homicide: A Sourcebook, edited by M.

    D. Smith and M. A. Zahn. Thousand Oaks, CA:

    Sage.

    Baldus, David C. and George Woodworth. 2003.

    “Race Discrimination in the Administration of the

    Death Penalty: An Overview of the Empirical

    Evidence with Special Emphasis on the Post-1990

    Research.” Criminal Law Bulletin 39:194–226.

    Bandes, Susan. 2004. “Fear Factor: The Role of

    Media in Covering and Shaping the Death Penalty.”

    Ohio State Journal of Criminal Law 1:585–97.

    Beckett, Katherine. 1997. Making Crime Pay. New

    York: Oxford.

    Berry, William D., Evan J. Ringquist, Richard C.

    Fording, and Russell L. Hanson. 1998. “Measuring

    Citizen and Government Ideology in the States.”

    American Journal of Political Science 42:327–48.

    Bickle, Gayle and Ruth Peterson. 1991. “The Impact

    of Gender Based Family Roles on Criminal

    Sentencing.” Social Problems 38:372–94.

    Blalock, Hubert M. 1967. Towards a Theory of

    Minority Group Relations. New York: Capricorn

    Books.

    Blume, J. and Theodore Eisenberg. 1999. “Judicial

    Politics, Death Penalty Appeals, and Case

    WHO SURVIVES ON DEATH ROW?—–629

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 629

    Selection.” Southern California Law Review

    72:465–503.

    Blumer, Herbert. 1958. “Race Prejudice as a Sense

    of Group Position.” Pacific Sociological Review

    1:3–7.

    Bobo, Lawrence and Vincent Hutchings. 1996.

    “Perceptions of Racial Group Competition:

    Extending Blumer’s Theory of Group Position in

    a Multiracial Social Context.” American

    Sociological Review 61:951–72.

    Brace, Paul and Melinda G. Hall. 1997. “The

    Interplay of Preferences, Case Facts, Context, and

    Rules in the Politics of Judicial Choice.” Journal

    of Politics 59:1206–23.

    Brennan, William J. 1987. Dissent in McClesky v.

    Kemp (481 U.S. 279, 343).

    Bright, Stephen B. and Patrick J. Keenan. 1995.

    “Judges and the Politics of Death: Deciding

    Between the Bill of Rights and the Next Election

    in Capital Cases.” Boston University Law Review

    75:759–835.

    Brown, Craig and Barbara War ner. 1992.

    “Immigrants, Urban Politics, and Policing in 1900.”

    American Sociological Review 57:293–305.

    Carter, Dan T. 1996. From George Wallace to Newt

    Gingrich: Race in the Conservative

    Counterrevolution, 1963–1994. Baton Rouge, LA:

    Louisiana State University Press.

    Chambliss, William J. 1994. “Policing the Ghetto

    Underclass: The Politics of Law and Law

    Enforcement.” Social Problems 41:177–94.

    Chiricos, Theodore G. and Charles Crawford. 1995.

    “Race and Imprisonment.” Pp. 281–309 in

    Ethnicity, Race, and Crime, edited by D. Hawkins.

    Albany, NY: SUNY Press.

    Chiricos, Theodore G. and Miriam Delone. 1992.

    “Labor Surplus and Punishment: A Review and

    Assessment of Theory and Evidence.” Social

    Problems 39:421–46.

    Cohen, Jacob, Patricia Cohen, Stephen D. West, and

    Leona S. Aiken. 2003. Applied Multiple

    Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral

    Sciences. 3d ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

    Associates.

    Constanzo, Mark. 1997. Just Revenge: The Costs

    and Consequences of the Death Penalty. New

    York: St. Martins Press.

    Dodge, Lowell, Laurie E. Eckland, Harriet C. Ganson,

    Lisa Cassady, James L. Flemming, and Douglas M.

    Sloan. 1990. “Death Penalty Sentencing.” Pp.

    268–74 in Death Penalty in America, edited by H.

    A. Bedau. New York: Oxford.

    Donohue, John J. and Justin Wolfers. 2005. “Uses and

    Abuses of Empirical Evidence in the Death Penalty

    Debate.” Stanford Law Review 58:791–845.

    Ehrlichman, John D. 1982. Witness to Power: The

    Nixon Years. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Fosset, Mark A. and K. Jill Kiecolt. 1989. “The

    Relative Size of Minority Populations and White

    Racial Attitudes.” Social Science Quarterly

    70:820–35.

    Foucault, Michel. 1977. Discipline and Punish.

    London: Vintage.

    Fowler, Linda L. 1982. “How Interest Groups Select

    Issues for Rating Voting Records of Members of

    the U.S. Congress.” Legislative Studies Quarterly

    7:401–13.

    Furman v. Georgia. 1972. 408 U.S. 238.

    Garland, David. 1990. Punishment and Modern

    Society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    ———. 2001. The Culture of Control. Chicago,

    IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Giles, Michael W. and Melanie A. Buckner. 1993.

    “David Duke and Black Threat.” Journal of Politics

    55:702–13.

    Giles, Michael W. and Kaenan Hertz. 1994. “Racial

    Threat and Partisan Identif ication.” American

    Political Science Review 88:317–26.

    Goldfield, Michael. 1997. The Color of Politics. New

    York: New Press.

    Gregg v. Georgia. 1976. 96 Sup. Ct. 2902.

    Hale, Chris. 1996. “Fear of Crime: A Review of the

    Literature.” International Review of Victimology

    4:79–150.

    Heer, David M. 1959. “The Sentiment of White

    Supremacy: An Ecological Study.” American

    Journal of Sociology 64:592–98.

    Helms, Ronald and David Jacobs. 2002. “The

    Political Context of Sentencing: An Analysis of

    Community and Individual Determinants.” Social

    Forces 81:577–604.

    Herrera, Richard, Thomas Epperlein, and Eric R. A.

    N. Smith. 1995. “The Stability of Congressional

    Roll-Call Indexes.” Political Research Quarterly

    48:403–16.

    Hood, Roger. 1998. “Capital Punishment.” Pp.

    739– 76 in The Handbo ok o f C r ime and

    Punishment, edited by M. Tonry. New York: Oxford

    University Press.

    Hurwitz, Jon and Mark Peffley. 2005. “Playing the

    Race Card in the Post-Willie Horton Era.” Public

    Opinion Quarterly 69:99–112.

    Jacobs, David. 1979. “Inequality and Police Strength:

    Conflict Theor y and Coercive Control in

    Metropolitan Areas.” American Sociological

    Review 44:913–25.

    Jacobs, David and Jason T. Carmichael. 2001. “The

    Politics of Punishment Across Time and Space.”

    Social Forces 80:61–89.

    ———. 2002. “The Political Sociology of the Death

    Penalty: A Pooled Time-Series Analysis.”

    American Sociological Review 67:109–31.

    Jacobs, David, Jason T. Carmichael, and Stephanie

    L. Kent. 2005. “Vigilantism, Current Racial Threat,

    and Death Sentences.” American Sociological

    Review 70:656–77.

    Jacobs, David and Ronald Helms. 1996. “Towards a

    Political Model of Incarceration: A Time-Series

    630—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 630

    Examination of Multiple Explanations for Prison

    Admission Rates.” American Journal of Sociology

    102:323–57.

    Jacobs, David and Robert M. O’Brien. 1998. “The

    Determinants of Deadly Force: A Structural

    Analysis of Police Violence.” American Journal of

    Sociology 103:837–62.

    Jacobs, David and Daniel B. Tope. 2007. “The Politics

    of Resentment in the Post Civil-Rights Era:

    Minority Threat, Homicide, and Ideological Voting

    in Congress.” American Journal of Sociology

    113:1458–94.

    Johnston, Jack. 1984. Econometric Methods. New

    York: McGraw-Hill.

    Kent, Stephanie L. and David Jacobs. 2005.

    “Minority Threat and Police Strength from 1980

    to 2000: A Fixed-Effects Analysis of Nonlinear and

    Interactive Effects in Large U.S. Cities.”

    Criminology 43:731–60.

    Key, V. O. 1949. Southern Politics. New York: Vintage

    Books.

    Lacey, Nicola. 1988. State Punishment. London:

    Routledge.

    Lakoff, George. 1996. Moral Politics. Chicago, IL:

    University of Chicago Press.

    Langworthy, Robert H. and John T. Whitehead. 1986.

    “Liberalism and Fear as Explanations for

    Punitiveness.” Criminology 24:575–91.

    Levitt, Steven D. 2002. “Deterrence.” Pp. 435–50 in

    Crime: Public Policies for Crime Control, edited

    by J. Q. Wilson and J. Petersillia. Oakland, CA: ICS

    Press.

    Liebman, James S., Jeffrey Fagan, and Valerie West.

    2000. “A Broken System: Error Rates in Capital

    Cases, 1973–1995.” Report, Columbia University

    Law School.

    ———. 2002. “A Broken System, Part II: Why

    There is so Much Error in Capital Cases, and What

    Can be Done About It.” Repor t, Columbia

    University Law School.

    Lipschultz, Jeremy H. and Michael L. Hilt. 2002.

    Crime and Local Television News: Dramatic,

    Breaking, and Live from the Scene. Mahwah, NJ:

    Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Liska, Allen E., J. J. Lawrence, and A. Sanchirico.

    1982. “Fear of Crime as a Social Fact.” Social

    Forces 60:760–71.

    Lofquist, William S. 2002. “Putting Them There,

    Keeping Them There, and Killing Them: An

    Analysis of State-Level Variations in Death Penalty

    Intensity.” Iowa Law Review 87:1505–57.

    Manski, Charles F. and Steven R. Lerman. 1977.

    “Estimation of Choice Probabilities from Choice

    Based Samples.” Econometrica 45:1977–89.

    Messner, Steven F., Robert D. Baller, and Matthew

    P. Zevenbergen. 2005. “The Legacy of Lynching

    and Southern Homicide.” American Sociological

    Review 70:633–55.

    Myers, Martha A. and Suzette M. Talarico. 1987.

    The Social Contexts of Criminal Sentencing. New

    York: Springer-Verlag New York Inc.

    Myrdal, Gunnar. 1944. An American Dilemma. New

    York: Harper and Brothers.

    Paternoster, Raymond. 1991. Capital Punishment in

    America. New York: Lexington Books.

    Perrow, Charles. 1986. Complex Organizations: A

    Critical Essay. New York: Random House.

    Poole, Keith T. and Howard Rosenthal. 2000.

    Congress: A Political-Economic History of Roll

    Call Voting. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Prentice, R. L. and R. Pyke. 1979. “Logistic Disease

    Incidence Models and Case-Control Studies.”

    Biometrika 66:403–11.

    Quillian, Lincoln. 1996. “Group Threat and Changes

    in Regional Change in Attitudes Toward African-

    Americans.” American Journal of Sociology

    102:816–60.

    Quillian, Lincoln and Devah Pager. 2001. “Black

    Neighbors, Higher Crime? The Role of Racial

    Stereotypes in Evaluations of Neighborhood

    Crime.” American Journal of Sociolog y

    107:717–67.

    Rogers, William H. 1993. “Regression Standard

    Errors in Clustered Samples.” Stata Technical

    Bulletin 13:1923–28.

    Rusche, Georg and Otto Kirchheimer. 1939.

    Punishment and Social Structure. New York:

    Russell and Russell.

    Savelsberg, Joachim J. 1994. “Knowledge,

    Domination, and Criminal Punishment.” American

    Journal of Sociology 99:911–43.

    Savelsberg, Joachim J. and Ryan D. King. 2005.

    “Institutionalizing Collective Memories of Hate:

    Law and Law Enforcement in Germany and the

    United States.” American Journal of Sociology

    111:579–616.

    Scott, W. Richard. 1987. Organizations: Rational,

    Natural, and Open Systems. Englewood Cliffs,

    NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Shaffer, William R. 1989. “Rating the Performance

    of the ADA in the U.S. Congress.” Western Political

    Quarterly 42:33–51.

    Spurr, Stephen J. 2002. “The Future of Capital

    Punishment: Determinants of the Time from Death

    Sentence to Execution.” International Review of

    Law and Economics 22:1–23.

    Stucky, Thomas D., Karen Heimer, and Joseph B.

    Lang. 2005. “Par tisan Politics, Electoral

    Competition, and Imprisonment.” Criminology

    43:211–48.

    Sutton, John. 2000. “Imprisonment and Social

    Classification in Five Common-Law Democracies,

    1955–1985.” American Journal of Sociology

    106:350–86.

    Taylor, Ian, P. Walton, and Jock Young. 1973. The New

    Criminology. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Taylor, Marylee C. 1998. “How White Attitudes Vary

    WHO SURVIVES ON DEATH ROW?—–631

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 631

    with the Racial Composition of Local Populations.”

    American Sociological Review 63:512–35.

    Thorne, Melvin J. 1990. American Conservative

    Thought Since World War II. New York: Greenwood

    Press.

    Tocqueville, Alexis de. 1948. Democracy in America.

    New York: Knopf.

    Turk, Austen. 1969. Criminality and the Legal Order.

    Chicago, IL: Rand McNally.

    Unifor m Crime Repor ts [United States].

    “Supplementary Homicide Reports, Various Years.”

    Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-University Consortium for

    Political and Social Research.

    United States Department of Justice, Bureau of

    Justice Statistics. 2003. “Capital Punishment in the

    United States, 1973–2002.” Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-

    University Consortium for Political and Social

    Research.

    Wacquant, Loic. 2000. “The New Par ticular

    Institution: On the Prison as Surrogate Ghetto.”

    Theoretical Criminology 4:377–89.———. 2001. “Deadly Symbiosis: When Ghetto

    and Prison Meet and Mesh.” Punishment & Society

    3:95–134.

    Walker, Samuel, Cassia Spohn, and Miriam DeLone.

    1996. The Color of Justice: Race, Ethnicity, and

    Crime in America. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

    Weidner, Robert R. and Richard S. Frase. 2003.

    “Legal and Extralegal Determinants of Intercounty

    Differences in Prison Use.” Criminal Justice Policy

    Review 14:377–400.

    Western, Bruce. 2006. Punishment and Inequality in

    America. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Whitman, James Q. 2003. Harsh Justice: Criminal

    Punishment and the Widening Divide Between

    America and Europe. New York: Oxford.

    Xie, Yu and Charles P. Manski. 1989. “The Logit

    Model and Response-Based Samples.”

    Sociological Methods and Research 17:283–302.

    Zatz, Marjorie S. 1987. “The Changing Forms of

    Racial/Ethnic Biases in Sentencing.” Journal of

    Research in Crime and Delinquency 24:69–92.

    Zimring, Franklin E. and Gordon Hawkins. 1986.

    Capital Punishment and the American Agenda.

    New York: Cambridge.

    632—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    #3154-ASR 72:4 filename:72406-jacobs page 632

                                                                                                                                      Order Now